Articles | Volume 4, issue 1
https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-1-2017
© Author(s) 2017. This work is distributed under
the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-1-2017
© Author(s) 2017. This work is distributed under
the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Age-dependent change of coalitionary strategy in male Barbary macaques
Eva-Maria Rathke
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR
Department of Behavioral Ecology, Georg-August University Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Cognitive Ethology Laboratory, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Andreas Berghänel
Department of Behavioral Ecology, Georg-August University Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, MSC01-1040, Anthropology, 1, Albuquerque, NM 87131, USA
Annie Bissonnette
Department of Behavioral Ecology, Georg-August University Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Anthropological Institute and Museum, University of Zürich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
Julia Ostner
Department of Behavioral Ecology, Georg-August University Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Oliver Schülke
Department of Behavioral Ecology, Georg-August University Göttingen, Kellnerweg 6, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Primate Social Evolution Group, German Primate Center, Leibniz Institute for Primate Research, Kellnerweg 4, 37077 Göttingen, Germany
Related authors
No articles found.
Anne Seltmann, Mathias Franz, Bonaventura Majolo, Mohamed Qarro, Julia Ostner, and Oliver Schülke
Primate Biol., 3, 23–31, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-23-2016, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-23-2016, 2016
Short summary
Short summary
We investigated collective movements in a group of wild Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) in the Middle Atlas, Morocco. While pauses of the first departing individual enhanced following behavior, the opposite was found for back glancing. To successfully initiate a collective movement, it seemed to be sufficient for a socially integrated group member to take action when other group members signal their willingness prior to departure and to occasionally wait for the group while moving.
Related subject area
Behaviour
Male-biased dominance in greater bamboo lemurs (Prolemur simus)
Report of an attack on a howler monkey Alouatta sara by a group of collared peccaries Dicotyles tajacu at a mammal clay lick in Madre de Dios, Peru
Fur rubbing in Plecturocebus cupreus – an incidence of self-medication?
Masturbation in a male Phayre's langur, Trachypithecus phayrei
Life on the edge: behavioural and physiological responses of Verreaux's sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi) to forest edges
Potential self-medication by brown titi monkeys, Plecturocebus brunneus, in an urban fragment forest in the Brazilian Amazon
Postural behavior of howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata, A. macconnelli, and A. caraya) during sleep: an assessment across the genus range
Active anti-predator behaviour of red titi monkeys (Plecturocebus cupreus)
Self-anointing behaviour in captive titi monkeys (Callicebus spp.)
Estimation of baboon daily travel distances by means of point sampling – the magnitude of underestimation
Fur-rubbing with Piper leaves in the San Martín titi monkey, Callicebus oenanthe
Olive baboons' (Papio anubis) response towards crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus) at Lake Manyara National Park
Female infanticide and female-directed lethal targeted aggression in a group of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta)
Meat sharing between male and female Guinea baboons (Papio papio)
Tree shrews at the German Primate Center
History of primate behavioural and ecological field research at the German Primate Center
Dry season drinking from terrestrial man-made watering holes in arboreal wild Temminck's red colobus, The Gambia
Do wild titi monkeys show empathy?
Lilith Sidler, Johanna Rode-White, and Peter M. Kappeler
Primate Biol., 11, 13–17, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-11-13-2024, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-11-13-2024, 2024
Short summary
Short summary
Reports of male dominance in greater bamboo lemurs were not supported by data. We studied agonistic behavior in one captive group and could confirm this assertion, which sets this species apart from all other group-living lemurs.
Raul Bello, Eckhard Heymann, and Sam Pottie
Primate Biol., 9, 29–31, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-9-29-2022, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-9-29-2022, 2022
Short summary
Short summary
A camera trap captured the moment in which a group of collared peccaries attacked and killed a howler monkey at a clay lick in the Taricaya Ecological Reserve, Madre de Dios, Peru. This novel behaviour helps us understand the myriad risks howler monkeys face when descending to the forest floor while also indirectly emphasizing the importance of geophagy for these animals.
Gurjit K. Theara, Juan Ruíz Macedo, Ricardo Zárate Gómez, Eckhard W. Heymann, and Sofya Dolotovskaya
Primate Biol., 9, 7–10, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-9-7-2022, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-9-7-2022, 2022
Short summary
Short summary
As part of our study on coppery titi monkeys in the Peruvian Amazon, we observed an individual rubbing his belly with leaves from a potentially medicinal plant. He climbed down from high above and grabbed a handful of leaves from an understory tree, which made it look like a deliberate action. Sitting on a liana he then chewed on the leaves before rubbing them slowly on his belly for a couple of minutes. The plant genus is known in traditional medicine and is used for various effects.
Md Shalauddin, Md Jayedul Islam, and Tanvir Ahmed
Primate Biol., 8, 43–45, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-8-43-2021, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-8-43-2021, 2021
Short summary
Short summary
We report the first masturbatory behaviour in a male Phayre's langur, in a semi-evergreen forest in Bangladesh. We discuss possible reasons for masturbation and suggest an adequate characterization of the socio-sexual behaviour and reproductive strategies of this globally endangered primate to demonstrate the causes, cost and consequences of masturbation. We also urge further scientific exploration into masturbation among primates due to its evolutionary and conservation significance.
Klara Dinter, Michael Heistermann, Peter M. Kappeler, and Claudia Fichtel
Primate Biol., 8, 1–13, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-8-1-2021, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-8-1-2021, 2021
Short summary
Short summary
We examined the influence of forest edge effects on activity budgets, feeding ecology, and stress hormone output in five groups of Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi) in western Madagascar. Sifakas in the edge habitat travelled more, tended to have smaller home ranges, had lower fruit consumption, higher stress hormone levels, and lower birth rates than sifakas in the forest interior. Hence, Verreaux’s sifakas appear to be sensitive to microhabitat characteristics linked to forest edges.
Brenda Letícia Pereira Oliveira, João Pedro Souza-Alves, and Marcela Alvares Oliveira
Primate Biol., 7, 35–39, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-7-35-2020, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-7-35-2020, 2020
Bernardo Urbani, Dionisios Youlatos, and Martín M. Kowalewski
Primate Biol., 7, 25–33, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-7-25-2020, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-7-25-2020, 2020
Short summary
Short summary
Sleep is the longest and most continuous behavioral phase in the 24 h cycle of mammals. However, selection of postures, substrates, and tree parts during sleep has not been adequately explored, as well as their evolutionary consequences. The present study investigates postural behavior, substrate, and tree part use during sleep in three howler species (A. palliata, A. macconnelli, and A. caraya) in Nicaragua, French Guiana, and Argentina. All three species were consistent in the use of a crouch.
Sofya Dolotovskaya, Camilo Flores Amasifuen, Caroline Elisabeth Haas, Fabian Nummert, and Eckhard W. Heymann
Primate Biol., 6, 59–64, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-6-59-2019, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-6-59-2019, 2019
Short summary
Short summary
Reactions to predators vary a lot in primates and can be passive (hiding, fleeing) or active (mobbing, alarm calls). Due to their secretive lifestyle, Neotropical titi monkeys are thought to use mainly passive crypsis and hiding as anti-predator responses. Predator mobbing has been reported only for one titi species, Callicebus nigrifrons. We report mobbing of an ocelot and Boa constrictor in red titi monkeys and Plecturocebus cupreus, and alarm calling as a reaction to tayras and raptors.
João Pedro Souza-Alves, Natasha M. Albuquerque, Luana Vinhas, Thayane S. Cardoso, Raone Beltrão-Mendes, and Leandro Jerusalinsky
Primate Biol., 5, 1–5, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-5-1-2018, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-5-1-2018, 2018
Short summary
Short summary
These data come from the major study on gut passage with two primate species (Callicebus coimbrai and Callicebus barbarabrownae) enclosed in the Zoobotanical Park localized in the north-eastern Brazil. During the sampling period, we have decided to obtain self-anointing and laterality data. Due to the presence of large glands in C. coimbrai and C. barbarabrownae chests, we cautiously suggest that the use of Bauhinia may be linked to olfactory communication.
Holger Sennhenn-Reulen, Langhalima Diedhiou, Matthias Klapproth, and Dietmar Zinner
Primate Biol., 4, 143–151, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-143-2017, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-143-2017, 2017
Rosario Huashuayo-Llamocca and Eckhard W. Heymann
Primate Biol., 4, 127–130, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-127-2017, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-127-2017, 2017
Short summary
Short summary
We report observations fur-rubbing with leaves from the spiked pepper plant, Piper aduncum, in the San Martín titi monkey, Callicebus oenanthe. As leaf extracts from this plant include insecticidal compounds, we interpret this behaviour as a defense against ectoparasites. Our observations expand the number of primate species for which this kind of self-medication is reported.
Filipa M. D. Paciência, Deusdedith Baluya, Pay Mbaryo, Sascha Knauf, and Dietmar Zinner
Primate Biol., 4, 101–106, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-101-2017, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-4-101-2017, 2017
Short summary
Short summary
Here we report two encounters between olive baboons (Papio anubis) and crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus), a potential predator, at Lake Manyara National Park, Tanzania. On both events, baboons gave alarm calls while infants got close to adults and juveniles ran to cover. Adult males approached the eagles and chased them away. The baboons’ reactions indeed support the assumption that crowned eagles pose a threat, at least for juvenile baboons.
Klara Kittler and Silvio Dietzel
Primate Biol., 3, 41–46, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-41-2016, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-41-2016, 2016
Short summary
Short summary
We report on extremely rare events of group eviction and eventually lethal aggression in a group of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) that were targeted against the two highest-ranking females. These events erupted after an infanticide by the highest-ranking female directed at the offspring of a subordinate. We suggest that this aggressive changeover of power between two matrilines was based on the growing group size and was an act of female reproductive competition during birth season.
A. S. Goffe and J. Fischer
Primate Biol., 3, 1–8, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-1-2016, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-3-1-2016, 2016
Short summary
Short summary
We describe the acquisition, sharing and consumption of meat by wild Guinea baboons. Males were the hunters and shared meat passively only with females in their social and reproductive units. Females were able to acquire meat from sharing episodes and by scavenging from their male social partners. The presence of existing social bonds, rather than short-term exchanges of meat for sex, is likely a driving factor in the occurrence of meat sharing between males and females in this species.
E. Fuchs
Primate Biol., 2, 111–118, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-111-2015, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-111-2015, 2015
E. W. Heymann and J. U. Ganzhorn
Primate Biol., 2, 73–80, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-73-2015, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-73-2015, 2015
A. P. Hillyer, R. Armstrong, and A. H. Korstjens
Primate Biol., 2, 21–24, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-21-2015, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-2-21-2015, 2015
Short summary
Short summary
Terrestrial drinking is not normally reported for arboreal primates. Here we report observations of terrestrial drinking from man-made watering holes by Temminck’s red colobus (Piliocolobus badius temminckii) at two sites in The Gambia. Our observations show that shallow man-made watering holes, not harbouring predators, were used by different age classes. The implications of this behaviour for this endangered subspecies and the trend of increasing temperatures in The Gambia are discussed.
A. Clyvia, M. C. Kaizer, R. V. Santos, R. J. Young, and C. Cäsar
Primate Biol., 1, 23–28, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-1-23-2014, https://doi.org/10.5194/pb-1-23-2014, 2014
Short summary
Short summary
We report the first case of putative empathic response in titi monkeys (Callibebus). Pair bonds between males and females are typically strong, with substantial time spent grooming and tail twining. In an intriguing and unexpected observation we recorded an injured adult out-group male travelling with a neighbouring group. The group appeared to adapt travel patterns to allow him to accompany them, provided pro-social behaviour such as grooming and tolerated his presence at their sleeping site
Cited articles
Alberts, S. C.: Magnitude and sources of variation in male reproductive performance, in: The Evolution of Primate Societies, edited by: Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., Chicago University Press, Chicago, USA, 412–431, 2012.
Alberts, S. C., Watts, H. E., and Altmann, J.: Queuing and queue-jumping: long-term patterns of reproductive skew in male savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus, Anim. Behav., 65, 821–840, 2003.
Alberts, S. C., Buchan, J. C., and Altmann, J.: Sexual selection in wild baboons: from mating opportunities to paternity success, Anim. Behav., 72, 1177–1196, 2006.
Altmann, J.: Observational study of behaviour: sampling methods, Behaviour, 49, 227–267, 1974.
Bercovitch, F. B.: Coalitions, cooperation and reproductive tactics among adult male baboons, Anim. Behav., 36, 1198–1209, 1988.
Berghänel, A., Schülke, O., and Ostner, J.: Coalition formation among Barbary macaque males: the influence of scramble competition, Anim. Behav., 80, 675–682, 2010.
Berghänel, A., Ostner, J., and Schülke, O.: Coalitions destabilize dyadic dominance relationships in male Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus), Behaviour, 148, 1256–1274, 2011.
Bissonnette, A., Lange, E., and Van Schaik, C. P.: A cardinal measure of competitive ability in Barbary macaque males (Macaca sylvanus), Ethology, 115, 671–681, 2009a.
Bissonnette, A., de Vries, H., and van Schaik, C. P.: Coalitions in male Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus: strength, success and rules of thumb, Anim. Behav., 78, 329–335, 2009b.
Bissonnette, A., Bischofberger, N., and van Schaik, C. P.: Mating skew in Barbary macaque males: the role of female mating synchrony, female behavior, and male-male coalitions, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 65, 176–182, 2011.
Bissonnette, A., Franz, M., Schülke, O., and Ostner, J.: Socioecology, but not cognition, predicts male coalitions across primates, Behav. Ecol., 25, 794–801, https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru054, 2014.
Bissonnette, A., Perry, S., Barrett, L., Mitani, J. C., Flinn, M., Gavrilets, S., and de Waal, F. B.: Coalitions in theory and reality: a review of pertinent variables and processes, Behaviour, 152, 1–56, 2015.
Connor, R. C., Smolker, R. A., and Richards, A. F.: Two levels of alliance formation among male bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.), P. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 89, 987–990, 1992.
de Turckheim, G. and Merz, E.: Breeding Barbary macaques in outdoor open enclosures, in: The Barbary Macaque: A case study in conservation, edited by: Fa, J. E., Plenum Press, New York, USA, 241–261, 1984.
de Villiers, M. S., Richardson, P. R., and Van Jaarsveld, A. S.: Patterns of coalition formation and spatial association in a social carnivore, the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus), J. Zool., 260, 377–389, 2003.
de Vries. H., Stevens, J., and Vervaecke, H.: Measuring and testing the steepness of dominance hierarchies, Anim. Behav., 71, 585–592, 2006.
Dubuc, C., Muniz, L., Heistermann, M., Engelhardt, A., and Widdig, A.: Testing the priority-of-access model in a seasonally breeding primate species, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 65, 1615–1627, 2011.
Green, M. L., Herzing, D. L., and Baldwin, J. D.: Reproductive success of male Atlantic spotted dolphins (Stenella frontalis) revealed by noninvasive genetic analysis of paternity, Can. J. Zool., 89, 239–253, 2011.
Grinnell, J.: Modes of cooperation during territorial defense by African lions, Hum. Nature, 13, 85–104, 2002.
Henzi, S. P., Weingrill, T., and Barrett, L.: Male behaviour and the evolutionary ecology of chacma baboons, S. Afr. J. Sci., 95, 240–242, 1999.
Hothorn, T., Hornik, K., van de Wiel, M. A., and Zeileis, A.: Implementing a class of permutation tests: The coin package, J. Stat. Softw., 28, 1–23, https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v028.i08, 2008.
IUCN: Red List of Threatened Species, Version 2015-4, available at: http://www.iucnredlist.org, last access: 14 July 2016.
Krützen, M., Sherwin, W. B., Connor, R. C., Barré, L. M., Van de Casteele, T., Mann, J., and Brooks, R.: Contrasting relatedness patterns in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) with different alliance strategies, P. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. Bio., 270, 497–502, 2003.
Kuester, J., Paul, A., and Arnemann, J.: Age-related and individual differences of reproductive success in male and female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus), Primates, 36, 461–476, 1995.
Martin, P. and Bateson, P. P. G.: Measuring behaviour: an introductory guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 1993.
Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., Pepper, J. W., and Merriwether, D. A.: Demographic and social constraints on male chimpanzee behaviour, Anim. Behav., 64, 727–737, 2002.
Noë, R.: Alliance formation among male baboons: shopping for profitable partners, in: Coalitions and alliances in humans and other animals, edited by: Harcourt, A. and de Waal, F., Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, 285–322, 1992.
Noë, R.: A model of coalition formation among male baboons with lighting ability as the crucial parameter, Anim. Behav., 47, 211–213, 1994.
Noë, R. and Sluijter, A. A.: Reproductive tactics of male savanna baboons, Behaviour, 113, 117–169, 1990.
Noë, R. and Sluijter, A. A.: Which adult male savanna baboons form coalitions?, Int. J. Primatol., 16, 77–105, 1995.
Ostner, J. and Schülke, O.: The evolution of social bonds in primate males, Behaviour, 151, 871–906, 2014.
Packer, C. and Pusey, A. E.: Cooperation and competition within coalitions of male lions: kin selection or game theory?, Nature, 296, 740–742, 1982.
Pandit, S. A. and van Schaik, C. P.: A model for leveling coalitions among primate males: toward a theory of egalitarianism, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 55, 161–168, 2003.
Preuschoft, S. and Paul, A.: Dominance, egalitarianism, and stalemate: An experimental approach to male-male competition in Barbary macaques, in: Primate males: Causes and consequences of variation in group composition, edited by: Kappeler, P. M., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 205–216, 2000.
R Core Team: R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, available at: http://www.R-project.org/ (last access: 23 June 2016), 2014.
Schülke, O., Bhagavatula, J., Vigilant, L., and Ostner, J.: Social bonds enhance reproductive success in male macaques, Curr. Biol., 20, 2207–2210, 2010.
Setchell, J.: Alternative reproductive tactics in primates, in: Alternative reproductive tactics: an integrative approach, edited by: Oliveira, R. F., Taborsky, M., and Brockmann H. J., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, 373–398, 2008.
Silk, J. B.: The patterning of intervention among male bonnet macaques: reciprocity, revenge, and loyalty, Curr. Anthropol., 33, 318–325, 1992.
Smith, J. E., Van Horn, R. C., Powning, K. S., Cole, A. R., Graham, K. E., Memenis, S. K., and Holekamp, K. E.: Evolutionary forces favoring intragroup coalitions among spotted hyenas and other animals, Behav. Ecol., 21, 284–303, 2010.
Smuts, B. B.: Sex and friendships in baboons, Aldine Publishing, New York, USA, 1985.
Smuts, B. B. and Watanabe, J. M.: Social relationships and ritualized greetings in adult male baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis), Int. J. Primatol., 11, 147–172, 1990.
Van Schaik, C. P., Pandit, S. A., and Vogel, E. R.: Toward a general model for male-male coalitions in primate groups, in: Cooperation in primates and humans, edited by: Kappeler, P. M. and van Schaik, C. P., Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, Germany, 151–171, 2006.
Wittig, R. M. and Boesch, C.: Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Tai National Park, Int. J. Primatol., 24, 847–867, 2003.
Young, C., Hähndel, S., Majolo, B., Schülke, O., and Ostner, J.: Male coalitions and female behaviour affect male mating success independent of dominance rank and female receptive synchrony in wild Barbary macaques, Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 67, 1665–1677, 2013.
Young, C., Majolo, B., Schülke, O., and Ostner, J.: How males form coalitions against group rivals and the Pandit/van Schaik coalition model, Behaviour, 151, 907–934, 2014a.
Young, C., Majolo, B., Schülke, O., and Ostner, J.: Male social bonds and rank predict supporter selection in cooperative aggression in wild Barbary macaques, Anim. Behav., 95, 23–32, 2014b.
Short summary
Coalitions are a specific case of cooperative competition and have been explained by various factors like partner availability, distribution of fighting ability, or cost of coordination. We investigated if age is an important factor influencing coalition formation and, specifically, if males switch from a solo strategy to a cooperative one after reaching post-prime age. We found that older males formed more coalitions than younger ones, stressing the importance of age in cooperative competition.
Coalitions are a specific case of cooperative competition and have been explained by various...